A look at some
psychological theories
The social nature of shame and guilt and their interpersonal origin have
been confirmed by some former theories, denied by others and tried counteracted
by most. Some have even tried to combine analysis of their important social
aspects, while at the same time denied such aspects. I have no good explanation
for these inconsistent findings, other than seeing that most of them are
created in an empirical vacuum. Many theories about shame and guilt are made
from intuition and from observations and clinical impressions that are not
collected systematically. On the other hand there are also studies that have so
much empirical data that, collecting information has been seen as more
important than the development of new theory.
1.Sigmund Freud (1856-1939) looked
at guilt and shame as products of intra psychic conflicts or more precisely as
a weapon used by the superego to influence decisions made by the ego, e.g. as
he writes in his book New Introductory
Lectures on Psychoanalysis (1933/1974) “…moral sense of guilt is the
expression of the tension between the ego and the superego” (p.76). He insisted
that the superego created guilt without any consideration to the outside world.
There are some traces of interpersonal relations in Freud. He writes e.g. in Civilization and its Discontent (1930/2005)
that the superego is a adaptation of the human organism with the civilised
world, which can be understood as living together with other people.
2.Helen Block Lewis (1913-1987)
asserted in her book Shame and Guilt in
Neurosis (1971) that interpersonal factors are irrelevant for shame and
guilt. She writes e.g. that “guilt is evoked only from within the self; it is
thus a personal reaction to an “objective act of transgression”” (p.84). Guilt
is not even come from a possible contact with another person e.g. a generalised
other of an internalised reference group. She therefore denies in her analysis
any significant role of interpersonal processes, even though many of her
arguments seem to incline such processes.
3.Other theorists have explicitly and most
firmly denied interpersonal aspects of shame and guilt. Gerhart Piers and M.
Singer (1953/1971) write that these emotions are drawn off of castration anxiety
and they treat them as reactions to the impulses from id; aggression,
destruction and sexuality (especially incest). They allege that genuine shame
and guilt are “experienced in solitude and contain no conscious or realistic
reference to an audience” (Shame and
Guilt: A Psychoanalytic and Cultural Study. p.68), and thereby deny any
form of interpersonal dimension.
4.James Gilligan (1976)
asserts that shame and guilt comes from a reaction from aggressive instincts
connected with the early stage in Freuds scheme; the
“oral-biting-cannibalistic-sadistic, anal-sadistic and phallic-competitive”
stage (Beyond Morality: Psychoanalytic
Reflections on Shame, Guilt and Love,
p.149). Gilligan also writes that punishment is an important aspect to shame
and guilt.
5.In the book Self-Consciousness and Social Anxiety (1980) Arnold H. Buss writes
that guilt is connected to the private self-reflection, which does not
necessarily include other persons or their perspectives. “Guilt is essentially
private. The best test of guilt is whether anyone else knows of the
transgression” (p.159). The fact that no one has to know about ones guilt, just
as with shame, confirms their intra psychic nature.
6.A behavioristic perspective is
maintained by D.L. Mosher in his article Interaction
of fear and guilt in inhibiting unacceptable behaviour from 1965. He writes
that “guilt may be defined as a generalized or expectancy for self-mediated
punishment for violating, anticipating the violation of, or failure to attain
internalized standards of proper behavior” (p.162). His reference to a
self-mediated punishment makes it clear that the focus for guilt is not from
other persons, but something negative that one does to one self. Guilt, and in
my opinion also shame, are in a behavioristic perspective a anticipation of
self injury, not a interpersonal phenomena. Such a definition does not give
room for interpersonal factors, except for the possibility that “internalised
standards” can be learned from others.
7.Otto Rank (1884-1939) was one of
Sigmund Freud’s closest colleagues for more than twenty years, but the writing
of The Trauma of Birth (1929/1994)
changed that. Rank suggested that shame and guilt have social roots and was he
first to suggest a pre-oedipal phase, a phase before the Oedipal-complex which
Freud had put forth. Rank started with standard psychoanalytic views around
shame and guilt, but his thoughts developed gradually into a theoretical
position which is unique within psychoanalytic psychology. He came to
understand these emotions as a product of a process of becoming an individual,
together with other individuals. These emotions come forth in the infantile
dependence of the mother and in fear and anxiety for losing this dependency;
they function as a force which continues in this relation. Freud explained tirelessly, that it was
the Oedipus complex which was the nucleus of the neurosis and the foundational
source of all art, myth, religion, philosophy, therapy – indeed of all human
culture and civilization. So great was this impact on Freud that he distanced
himself from his colleague together with several others friends he had in the
Vienna Psychoanalytic Society, where Rank was Vice-President until then.
8.Already in 1937 did Karen Horney
(1885-1952), a pioneering theorist in personality, psychoanalysis, and
"feminine psychology", write in her book The Neurotic Personality of Our Time, that the feeling of guilt
(and shame) came from a basic fear for the missing approval from others, and
she speculated in that fact that we try to make others feel guilty (and
shameful) comes from neurotic interpersonal motives. With this, she turned
traditional psychoanalytic theory “upside down”. Horney often criticized the
work of Sigmund Freud. For instance, she opposed Freud's notion of penis envy,
claiming that what Freud was really detecting was women's justified envy of
men's power in the world. In her personality theory, Horney reformulated
Freudian thought and presented a holistic, humanistic perspective that
emphasized cultural and social influences, human growth, and the achievement of
self-actualization. Though she was often considered to be too outspoken, Horney
often has the distinction of being the only woman whose theory is included in
personality textbooks (Quinn, 1987).
A look at some
sociological theories
Jan E. Stets and Jonathan H. Thurner (2006) edited a useful reference
book called Handbook of the Sociology of
Emotions. One of the four sections in this book consists of a description
of thirteen sociological theories of emotional dynamics that have informed
empirical work. Several of these also include theories of shame and guilt.
1.Theordore D. Kemper (2006) writes
in this book of one of these theories which he calls “the power-status theory
of emotions”. He writes that we feel shame/embarrassment when one senses that
one’s own status is excessive and builds his theory, when it comes to guilt and
shame, on the works of Erving Goffman (1922-1982), especially The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life (1959) and Behavior in Public Places (1963). When one has done something one is
ashamed of, the result is losing honour. The way out of ones shame is not
through punishment, but through compensation, i.e.
“…an act or actions that reinstate
the person as one who deserves the amount of status originally claimed
originally that has been lost. Thus, if someone acts in a cowardly manner and
has thus brought shame on himself or herself, the solution usually is to engage
in immoderately risky behavior to show that the act of cowardice was an
aberration and not characteristic (p.100).
When the status of someone else is insufficient, this is
“because one is not conferring it in
adequate amounts. This can lead either to guilt
or
shame/embarrassment, or both. If the reason for the deprivation of the other is a power
tactic by the self, it will lead to guilt…If the reason, on the other hand, for
the deprivation is an inadequacy of the self, then the emotion is
shame/embarrassment” (p.101).
Guilt, in this theory, is “concerned
with doing wrong to another via excess power, frequently in violation of a
moral standard” (p.100). One feels guilt because of a wrong doing which makes
one feel that “one does not deserve to receive the amount of status one has
claimed for oneself” (p.100). A person can feel both shame and guilt in the
same in the same situation, but it is according to Kemper important to keep
them separate. They come from different forms of relationships and how to cope
with them will also differ.
2.Gretchen Peterson (2006) has
written about “the cultural theory” and argues that Charles Horton Cooley
(1864-1929) has shown us the link between emotions and the self. In his
conception of the looking-glass self, which he writes about in Human Nature and the Social Order
(1902/2006), he links emotional reactions to the conception of the self. Cooley
argued that shame come from how people perceive themselves is dependent upon
how they think they appear to others and how others are believed to judge that
appearance. The work of Cooley has set the stage for later theories on how
culture effects both emotions and the self.
3.In the book Social Evolution (1985), Robert L. Trivers writes from an
“evolutionistic perspective” that human guilt and shame comes from a natural
selection because these emotions prevent humans from carrying out actions that
could harm their relations to others. This is because such relations are
important for survival and reproduction. Michael Hammond (2006) writes that
“evolutionary theory” started with Charles Darwin’s (1809-1882) interest in how
emotions can be understood as a basic form for communication among humans and
animals (1872/2007). Paul Ekman (2004) has used a lifetime doing research on
how face expressions are used in all kinds of social interaction. He meant that
research of the face and emotions can cast light on the evolutionary origin of
emotions that are common in all human cultures. Hammond (2006) writes that for the
evolutionary thinker, emotions sort out different kind of behavior into
“meaningful categories that carry
rewards for following one path of action as opposed
to another. Emotions provide the
physiological impact that can give real weight to a conscience of a moral
imperative. Shame and guilt are the prime examples of this moralizing role for
emotions. They can give gravity to any social construction to which individuals
are emotionally tied. They can transform an ultimately arbitrary rule of
behavior into something that appears very meaningful to the individual bathed
in the emotional release tied to obeying or disobeying that rule” (p.370).
4.Turner (2006) writes that Thomas
J. Scheff (1988, 1994, 1997) put together the thoughts of Charels H. Cooley
(1902/2006) and Helen B. Lewis (1971) and came out with a new theory of
emotions. Scheff’s theory is characterized by is combination of psychoanalytic
tradition with symbolic interactionism. Scheff argues that shame is a repressed
emotion; we have a tendency not to show our shame so that shame is almost
invisible in our western culture. Gershen Kaufman (1980/1992) puts this view it
to the point by saying that
“our culture is a shame-based culture, but here, shame is hidden. There is shame about shame and so it remains under strict
taboo. Other cultures, for example, Eastern and Mediterranean,
are organized more openly around shame and its counterpart, honor. What we need
in our culture is to honor shame, and thereby redeem it” (p. 32-33).
Scheff and Retzinger (1991) write that our society, which represses
shame, is characterized by meetings between people where shame is not
acknowledged. Many people deny having shame over a whole lifetime. This
repression of shame in our society leads to a diffuse hostility which can be
used and/or misused by political leaders, such as Hitler before and under World
War II. Scheff characterizes shame as the master emotion as Lewis also coded
shame rather than guilt as the most common emotion. Turner (2006) calls
repression the master defence mechanism.
“The more negative the emotion and
the more they are associated with a failure to
verify self, the more probable is
repression…Most important, the more emotions are repressed, the more they will
be transmuted into new kinds of emotional response” (p.286).
References:
Buss, Arnold
H., 1980. Self-Consciousness and Social
Anxiety. W. F. Freeman
Cooley, Charles Horton, [1902] 2006. Human
Nature and the Social Order. New
Brunswick:
Transaction Publishers.
Darwin, Charles, [1872] 2007. The
Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. FQ
Classics.
Ekman, Paul, (2004). Emotions
Revealed: Recognizing Faces and Feelings to Improve
Communication and Emotional Life. Owl Books.
Freud, Sigmund [1933] 1974. New
Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis. Vintage/Edury
(Random House Group).
Freud, Sigmund, [1930] 2005. Civilization
and its Discontent. W.W. Norton & Company.
Gilligan, James, 1976. Beyond
Morality: Psychoanalytic Reflections on Shame, Guilt and
Love, in Moral Development and Behavior:
Theory, Research and Social Issues,
Thomas Lickona (ed.): New York: Holt: Rinehart
and Winston.
Goffman, Erving, [1959] 1990. The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. London: Penguin
Books.
Goffman, Erving, [1963] 1966. Behavior in Public Places. New
York: The Free Press.
Hammond, Michael, 2006. Evolutionary Theory and Emotions,
in Stets, Jan E. and
Jonathan
H. Turner (eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions.
Springer.
Horney, Karen, [1937] 1994. The Neurotic
Personality of Our Time. W.W. Norton &
Company.
Kaufman, Gershen, [1982] 1992. Shame
and the Power f Caring. Rochester:
Schenkman
Books Inc.
Kemper, Theordore D., 2006. Power and Status and the Power-Status Theory
of Emotions, in
Stets, Jan E. and Jonathan H. Turner
(eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of
Emotions. Springer.
Mosher, D. L., 1965. Interaction of fear and guilt in inhibiting
unacceptable behavior, in
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29: 161-167.
Peterson, Gretchen, 2006. Cultural Theory and Emotions, in Stets, Jan E.
and Jonathan H.
Turner (eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions. Springer.
Piers, Gerhart and M. Singer [1953] 1971. Shame and Guilt: A Psychoanalytic and Cultural
Study. New York: Charles C.
Thomas.
Quinn, S. (1987). A Mind of Her Own: The Life of Karen Horney. New York: Summit
Books.
Rank, Otto, [1929] 1994. The
Trauma of Birth. Dover
Publications.
Scheff, Thomas J., 1988. Shame and Conformity: The Deference-Emotion
System, in
American Sociological Review, 53: 395-406.
Sheff, Thomas J., 1994. Bloody
Revenge: Emotions, Nationalism and War. Boulder:
Westview Press.
Scheff, Thomas J., 1997. Emotions,
the Social Bond, and Human Reality. New
York:
Cambridge University Press.
Scheff, Thomas J. and Suzanne M. Retzinger, 1991. Emotions and Violence: Shame and Rage
in Destructive Conflicts. Lexington: Lexington.
Stets, Jan E. and Jonathan H. Turner (eds.), 2006. Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions.
Springer.
Trivers, Robert L., 1985. Social
Evolution. Benjamin-Cummings Pub Co.
Turner, Jonathan H., 2006. Psychoanalytic Sociological Theories and
Emotions, in Stets, Jan
E. and Jonathan H. Turner (eds.). Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions.
Springer.
very nice superbly explains each and every things
ReplyDelete